Social behavior has been predicted to select for increased neural investment (the social brain hypothesis) and also to select for decreased neural investment (the distributed cognition hypothesis). Here, we use two related bees, the social Augochlorella aurata (Smith) (Hymenoptera: Halictidae) and the related Augochlora pura (Say), which has lost social behavior, to test the contrasting predictions of these two hypotheses in these taxa. We measured the volumes of the mushroom body (MB) calyces, a brain area shown to be important for cognition in previous studies, as well as the optic lobes and antennal lobes. We compared females at the nest foundress stage when both species are solitary so that brain development would not be influenced by social interactions. We show that the loss of sociality was accompanied by a loss in relative neural investment in the MB calyces. This is consistent with the predictions of the social brain hypothesis. Ovary size did not correlate with MB calyx volume. This is the first study to demonstrate changes in mosaic brain evolution in response to the loss of sociality.
Because of their diversity of societies, social insects are excellent subjects for understanding how social behavior affects brain evolution (reviewed in Lihoreau et al. 2012, Farris 2016, O'Donnell and Boulva 2017, Godfrey and Gronenberg 2019). The ‘social brain hypothesis’ posits that the complexities of social interactions select for increased neural investment in cognition (Dunbar 1992, 2009; Dunbar and Schultz 2007; but see DeCasien et al. 2017; Kverková et al. 2018). In applying the social brain hypothesis to the Hymenoptera (the bees, ants, and wasps), Gronenberg and Riveros (2009) predicted that in the initial stages of social evolution characterized by small groups, cognitive demand would increase, as individuals had to contend with the same challenges as solitary species in addition to the complexities of social interactions. However, in larger-colony species with specialized division of labor, cognitive demand would decrease, as each individual would only be required to perform a subset of tasks (Gronenberg and Riveros 2009, Riveros et al. 2012, Godfrey and Gronenberg 2019).
The area of the insect brain that has received the most focus in studies of social insect brain evolution is the mushroom bodies (MB), which are paired neuropils involved in learning, memory, and sensory integration (Fahrbach 2006). The evolutionary origin of elaborate MBs in the Hymenoptera was apparently driven by the demands of foraging for a host that arose with the evolution of parasitoid wasps, which predates the evolution of nesting behavior and sociality in the aculate Hymenoptera (Farris and Schulmeister 2011, Farris 2016). However, recent intraspecific comparisons in social insects suggest that while the evolutionary origins of large MBs may predate sociality, differences in social behavior can still lead to differences in MB investment. For example, differences in MB volume associated with dominance status within social species demonstrate a social influence on MB size (Molina et al. 2007, 2008; O'Donnell et al. 2008, 2017; Smith et al. 2010; Rehan et al. 2015; Jaumann et al. 2019; Pahlke et al. 2019). A direct test of whether social behavior leads to increased MB investment relative to solitary living should compare closely related social and solitary species. While many studies have compared the MBs of individuals within social insect species (reviewed in Fahrbach 2006, Lihoreau et al. 2012, Farris 2016, O'Donnell and Boulva 2017, Godfrey and Gronenberg 2019), only one study to date directly compared a social group (the paper wasps, Vespidae, Polistinae) with the most closely related solitary subfamily (the potter wasps, Vespidae, Eumeninae) (O'Donnell et al. 2015). O'Donnell et al. (2015) found the opposite pattern to that predicted by the social brain hypothesis: the solitary potter wasps had larger MBs than the social paper wasps, although the differences were not statistically significant after controlling for phylogeny because their sample included only one evolutionary origin of sociality. O'Donnell et al. (2015) proposed that the social species benefit from ‘distributed cognition’ (Zhang and Norman 1994): the spreading of cognitive effort across the group through cooperation and task specialization. O’Donnell et al. (2015) argued that the cognitive benefits of task specialization were present even in small social groups, rather than only larger social insect colonies with more specialized division of labor, and that because early insect societies were family groups, kin selection reduced the conflict associated with sociality. Because neural tissue is expensive (Niven and Laughlin 2008), social insects should be selected to reduce MB size if not necessary due to distributed cognition.
A complicating factor in comparing social and solitary insect MBs is that social interactions themselves, as well as general foraging experience, may affect MB size. Drosophila reared in groups had larger MBs than those raised alone (Heisenberg et al. 1995), and Camponotus ants reared in isolation had smaller MBs than those left in their nest (Seid and Junge 2016). MBs may also show experience-dependent plasticity, increasing in volume in response foraging experience (Withers et al. 1993, 1995, 2008; Gronenberg et al. 1996; Farris et al. 2001; Kuhn-Buhlman and Wehner 2006; Maleszka et al. 2009; Molina and O'Donnell 2008; Seid and Wehner 2008; Stieb et al. 2010; Jones et al. 2013; Amador-Vargas et al. 2015; Rehan et al. 2015).
Here, we use an evolutionary loss of sociality to test whether there is a difference in the size of the MB calyces, the area of the brain found to differ between social and solitary paper wasps (O'Donnell et al. 2015), between a social species and its derived solitary relative. We use a social bee, Augochlorella aurata, and the closely related, sympatric solitary species from its sister genus (Goncalves 2016) Augochlora pura, to test if differences between these taxa are consistent with the contrasting predictions of the social brain or distributed cognition hypotheses. Augochlora pura and Augochlorella aurata share a common social ancestor, but A. pura has lost sociality (Stockhammer 1966, Ordway 1966, Mueller 1996, Danforth and Eickwort 1997, Dalmazzo and Roig-Alsina 2015). We compared the two species at the nest foundress stage in early summer, when they both exhibit solitary behavior because the workers of A. aurata have not yet eclosed. At this time, solitary foundresses of both species are foraging to provision their first generation of offspring (Ordway 1966, Mueller 1996). This ensures that any differences in neural investment are not in response to differences in foraging experience or the interactions that an A. aurata queen has with her workers. Differences in MB investment should thus reflect species-specific differences rather than adult experience. The social brain hypothesis predicts that the social species, A. aurata, should have larger MB calyces than the solitary A. pura, whereas the distributed cognition hypothesis predicts that the loss of sociality in A. pura will be accompanied by an increase in MB calyx volume. This is the first study to test whether the loss of sociality influences neural investment, and the first to compare neural investment in a social and solitary species before individuals are part of a social group.
Materials and Methods
Life History of Study Species
Augochlorella aurata (= A. striata) and Augochlora pura are both generalist foragers sympatric in the eastern United States. Females initiate nests in the late spring and early summer after emerging from winter diapause (Ordway 1966, Stockhammer 1966, Mueller 1996). In both species, the foundress females provision first brood offspring. The first brood A. aurata daughters remain in the nest as nonreproductive workers (average of four workers; Mueller 1991) with undeveloped ovaries, whereas A. pura offspring disperse and initiate their own nests. Augochlorella aurata queens do not leave the nest to forage after their workers have emerged; thus, A. aurata caught at flowers with enlarged reproductive ovaries represent foundresses whose (worker) offspring have not yet matured (Mueller 1996). Beyond their social behavior, the only notable difference in their ecology is that A. pura excavate nests in the rotting wood of fallen logs, whereas A. aurata excavate nests in the ground.
Collections
We collected seven female foundresses each of A. pura and A. aurata foraging on flowers in Montgomery Co. MD, Fairfax Co. VA, and Washington, DC, from 4 to 14 June 2017; one individual of A. pura was collected on 29 June 2017, and from 12 April to 15 June 2018. We immediately placed bees into 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffered saline (PBS) and stored them at 4°C until species identification. We measured ovary size because this correlated with MB volume in some previous studies (Molina et al. 2007, Rehan et al. 2015). We dissected away the tergites to photograph the ovaries dorsally at 10× magnification. We measured ovary size by tracing the outline of their photograph using ImageJ software following Smith et al. (2010). We used head width, measured with digital calipers, as a measure of body size.
Brain Analyses
Head capsules were dissected in PBS to remove brains and fixed in 4% PFA. Brains were then placed in a postfix of glutaraldehyde (2%), and dehydrated in a series of ethanol washes. Brains were mounted in methyl salicylate and visualized with autofluorescence following McKenzie et al. (2016) using an Olympus Fluoview FV1000 laser confocal microscope at 10× magnification (Fig. 1). The brain visualization used the 405-nm laser to enhance contrast (seen as blue) and the 488-nm (seen as green) to autofluoresce the glutaraldehyde. Images were optically sectioned at 4.27 µm until the entire brain was imaged in series. The PC-based software Reconstruct was used to quantify volumes of the MB calyces, optic lobes (OL), antennal lobes (AL), and the whole brain by tracing one side of each brain and extrapolating the total volumes for each brain section (Fiala 2005). All traces were done by MAS.
We measured volumes for the whole brain, MB calyces, the OL (including the lamina and medulla), and AL. We calculated ratios for each subregion of the brain relative to the whole brain to compare neural investment. To test whether brain size relative to body size differed between species, we standardized individual whole brain volume to individual body size by calculating a correction factor that was applied to each bee: mean body size of all bees in the study divided by the individual's body size. This correction factor was then multiplied to brain volume for each bee to calculate size-corrected whole brain volume (Jaumann et al. 2019). We used an independent samples t-test with equal variances not assumed to compare the two species; all variables fit a normal distribution with Shapiro–Wilk test P-values >0.05.
Results
Morphological Measurements
Augochlorella pura (mean head width ± SD = 2.26 ± 0.17 mm) were larger than A. aurata (2.04 ± 0.08 mm; t8.41 = 3.00, P = 0.016; Fig. 2a). Mean ovary size did not differ between species (A. pura: 0.59 ± 0.34 mm2, A. aurata: 0.85 ± 0.33 mm2, t11.97 = 1.45, P = 0.172; Fig. 2b), nor did it correlate with body size either across species (r = 0.30, n = 14, P = 0.30) or within species (A. aurata: r = –0.63, n = 7, P = 0.13; A. pura: r = 0.18, n = 7, P = 0.71).
Fig. 1.
Confocal image of Augochlora pura (left) and Augochlorella aurata (right) showing the brain areas used for analyses, the optic lobes, AL and MB calyces. Scale bars = 200 µm. The brain visualization used the 405 nm laser to enhance contrast (seen as blue) and the 488 nm (seen as green) to autofluoresce the glutaraldehyde.

Fig. 2.
Morphology data for the bees in our study, including head width (a), ovary area (b), whole brain volume (c), MB calyces volume (d), optic lobes volume, (e) and AL volume (f). Filled boxes represent Augochlorella aurata, open boxes Augochlora pura. Open circles are individual data points. Upper and lower bounds of boxes are one interquartile range (IQR) above and below the median. Error bars represent data within 1.5 (IQR) of the medial. Asterisks indicate statistically significant differences of P < 0.05 (*) or P < 0.01 (**).

Brain Measurements
Without accounting for body size, the larger species, A. pura, had larger brains than A. aurata (A. pura: 0.22 ± 0.03 mm3, A. aurata: 0.17 ± 0.03 mm3, t11.63 = 3.41, P = 0.005; Fig. 2c), although MB calyx volume did not differ (A. pura: 0.012 ± 0.001 mm3, A. aurata: 0.014 ± 0.003 mm3, t7.69 = 1.53, P = 0.165; Fig. 2d). Both AL and OL were larger in A. pura, but the difference in OL was not significant (AL: A. pura: 0.007 ± 0.001 mm3, A. aurata: 0.005 ± 0.001 mm3, t10.74 = 4.07, P = 0.002; OL:: A. pura: 0.042 ± 0.005 mm3, A. aurata: 0.034 ± 0.008 mm3, t11.14 = 2.163, P = 0.053; Fig. 2e and f).
Body size-corrected whole brain volume was larger in the solitary A. pura than the social A. aurata, but not significantly so (A. pura: 0.206 ± 0.04 mm3, A. aurata: 0.172 ± 0.024 mm3, t9.90 = 1.92, P = 0.084; Fig. 3d). Augochlorella aurata showed more investment in MB calyces than A. pura (A. aurata MB calyces:whole brain ratio = 0.080 ± 0.008, A. pura = 0.053 ± 0.006; t11.14 = 6.88, P < 0.001; Fig. 3a). All of the A. aurata individuals had larger MB calyces, relative to whole brain volume, than all of the A. pura individuals. The ratio of AL: whole brain volume did not differ between species (A. aurata = 0.028 ± 0.008, A. pura = 0.033 ± 0.005; t10.93 = 1.62, P = 0.133; Fig. 3b). Nor did the ratio of OL: whole brain volume differ between species (A. aurata = 0.204 ± 0.056, A. pura = 0.189 ± 0.019; t7.34 = 1.62, P = 0.538; Fig. 3c). Neither body size nor ovary size correlated with any of the neuropil: whole brain ratios in either of the two species.
Fig. 3.
Ratios of MB calyces to whole brain volume (a), AL to whole brain volume (b), and OL to whole brain volume (c) for the social species Augochlorella aurata (filled boxes) and the solitary species Augochlora pura (open boxes). Panel (d) shows body size-corrected (SC) whole brain volume for each species. Open circles are individual data points. Upper and lower bounds of boxes are one interquartile range (IQR) above and below the median. Error bars represent data within 1.5(IQR) of the median. Asterisks (**) indicate statistically significant differences of P < 0.01.

Discussion
Here we show that a social species, A. aurata, has larger MB calyces relative to brain size than the closely related A. aurata, which has lost sociality. This is consistent with predictions of the social brain hypothesis. These brain differences do not result from differences in social experience, because all individuals were solitary at the time they were collected, which suggests that the MB calyx size differences reflect species-specific patterns of neural investment. This is consistent with previous studies of socially polymorphic bees (those that can nest socially and solitarily) which also found smaller relative MB calyx size in solitary reproductives relative to social queens, but these did not control for potential effects of social interactions on MB size (Smith et al. 2010, Rehan et al. 2015; but see Jaumann et al. 2019).
Our MB calyx volume results show the opposite trend as the only other study to directly compare social species with closely related solitary species (O'Donnell et al. 2015). This may be due to the small size of A. aurata colonies, as O'Donnell et al. (2015) studied wasps with colonies of 20–4,000 workers, which even at the lower end is substantially larger than a typical A. aurata colony comprised of a queen and approximately four workers (Mueller et al. 1991). Perhaps the benefits of distributed cognition do not emerge until colony size is larger. Also, both our study and O'Donnell et al. (2015) examined only a single evolutionary transition between solitary and social behavior. Comparisons of multiple transitions are required before it will be clear if there is a general pattern of greater or less MB investment in social species. Halictid bees show multiple evolutionary gains and especially losses of sociality (Wcislo and Danforth 1997; Danforth et al. 1999, 2002; Danforth 2003; Gibbs et al. 2012) among species that are otherwise generally ecologically similar. Further studies of MB size across these multiple transitions can reveal whether social behavior is generally associated with increased or decreased neural investment in MBs or if differences result from lineage-specific factors unrelated to social behavior.
Acknowledgments
Christopher Day and Stephanie Keer assisted with confocal microscopy. This work was supported by National Science Foundation grant #17-1028536545 to A. R. S. and M. A. S. S. P. was supported by the Harlan Family Foundation and the Washington Biologists Field Club.