Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 April 2009 How Fledglings Explore Surroundings from Fledging to Dispersal. A Case Study with Eagle Owls Bubo bubo
María del Mar Delgado, Vincenzo Penteriani, Vilis O. Nams
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Movement strategies are one of the primary mechanisms underlying animal survival. Despite the impressive amount of studies on animal movement during successive stages in the life history in birds, little is known about the characteristics of movements during the post-fledging dependence period. Such a period represents a crucial phase of the life history during which individuals can show important displacements around the natal nest. Mere we present path search strategies employed by radio-tagged fledgling Eagle Owls Bubo bubo during the post-fledging dependence period (1962 locations from 41 tagged owlets). During the post-fledging dependence period, individuals show dynamic movement behaviours that might be related to the development of individuals' mobility and cognitive abilities. Immediately after leaving the nest, individuals focused their activities close to the nest, but after a few weeks individuals travelled further and faster, up to the moment that they started to disperse. At the time that the mobility of owlets was highest, their movements described straighter paths, Movement patterns were not affected by morphological- or physiological parameters of body condition. Between-sibling distances showed a significant interaction between sex and time: until 20 days after owlets left the nest, siblings were closer together, independently of the sex. Between-sibling distances increased with time as individuals became increasingly mobile; just before the start of dispersal, family units seemed to dissolve.

INTRODUCTION

The study of animal movement patterns allows us to understand ecological processes at various spatial and temporal scales (Kareiva & Shigesada 1983, Wiens et al. 1995, Blackwell 1997). At small spatial scales, animals rely on movements for finding food, mates, cover and avoiding predators (e.g. Vasquez et al. 2002). The way in which animals move around affects the fate of individuals and, consequently, the spatial structure, demographics and genetic structure of animal populations (Turchin 1998). As a consequence, in a still higher organizational level, movements affect population dynamics, community structures, and ecosystem biodiversity. Therefore, movements have important consequences at different ecological levels and scales for a variety of ecological processes and patterns (Crist et al. 1992, Wiens et al. 1995, Goodwing & Farhig 2002).

Until now, most movement studies have focused on: (a) movements within the home range (Worton 1987, Sunde & Redpath 2006), (b) search strategies directed toward favourable habitat patches (Zollner & Lima 1999), (c) the response to fragmented habitat (Crist et al. 1992, With & Crist 1995, Wiens et al. 1997), and (d) foraging movements in heterogeneous landscapes (Chris et al. 2002, Bailey & Thompson 2006). Due to the impact of floaters on breeding (meta) population dynamics (Heino et al. 1997, Hanski 1999, Penteriani et al. 2005a,b, 2006), dispersal has also become the focus of much current research on animal movements (Bowler & Benton 2005). However, few studies have analysed the temporal and spatial movement patterns before dispersal.

In birds, the period from fledging to independence is known as the post-fledging dependence period (hereafter, PFDP). During this stage, juveniles gradually have to: (1) enhance muscular development, (2) experiment with the external world and conspecifics without the protection of the nest, and (3) learn essential skills to survive as an adult. The different behavioural options of search strategy in this critical period might affect the fate of individuals. This period is a crucial phase in life history because it represents the time during which individuals reach the necessary body condition for dispersal. Reproductive efforts invested by parents during the previous stages may be lost if the PFDP occurs in hostile and uncertain environments or conditions. The PFDP is also a dynamic phase, in which juveniles increase their mobility and parental protection decreases (Bogner & Baldassarre 2002, Kershner et al. 2004, Myers & Vaughan 2004, Penteriani et al. 2005c, Wiens et al. 2006). If the increased travel occurs in hostile or unknown habitats, mortality could increase due to, for example, predation (Sunde 2005), electrocution (Sergio et al. 2004), as well as starvation and diseases (Aebischer et al. 2005). This increase in juvenile mortality may affect breeding populations because of the reduction of the floater segment of a population (Delibes et al. 2001, Penteriani et al. 2005a,b, 2006).

Despite the importance of the PFDP, information on this phase is scarce and is considered one of the missing links in our understanding of avian life histories (Sullivan 1989, Weatherhead & McRae 1990, Rappole 1995). The few studies analysing the PFDP have mainly focused on survival (e.g. Monrós et al. 2002, Sunde 2005, Hylton et al. 2006, Adams et al. 2006) and the length of the dependence period (Ferrer 1992, Amar et al. 2000, Mínguez et al. 2001). Little is known about the spatial and temporal movement patterns of juveniles and, although some studies have measured movements, they mainly focused on displacements from the nest or on the area used by juveniles (Kershner et al. 2004, Myers & Vaughan 2004, McClaren et al. 2005, Penteriani et al. 2005c, Wiens et al. 2006). However, knowledge of movement patterns during PFDP could be one of the most useful tools to allow one to better understand this final phase of the breeding cycle, as well as to be aware of the possible risks that the fledglings are confronted with. This is particularly important in the case of endangered species (as many raptor species), who during their exploratory PFDP moves may be faced with various stochastic events.

The main objective of our study was to describe and analyse the patterns of PFDP movements of fledgling Eagle Owls Bubo bubo, mainly focusing on: (1) the essential characteristics of individual movements, family unit interactions and territory use throughout the PFDP; (2) how movement patterns change during this stage of the breeding cycle; and (3) potential interactions between fledglings' physical conditions and features of movement patterns. In particular, we tested if individuals in better physical condition changed movement patterns sooner than other individuals.

METHODS

Study area and study species

We conducted field research from late March to early August 2004 and 2005 on 41 Eagle Owl fledglings (24 males; 17 females) from 13 different nests located in the Sierra Norte of Seville (37°30′N, 06°03′W, SW Spain; more details in Penteriani et al. 2005c).

The Eagle Owl, the largest owl in Europe, is a sexually plumage-monomorphic and socially monogamous long-lived species. It is a sedentary and territorial owl, with a high reproductive rate (Penteriani 1996). It is a generalist both in diet (Mikkola 1994, Lourenço 2006) and nest choice (Mikkola 1994, Penteriani et al. 2001, 2002, Marchesi et al 2002, Martinez et al. 2003), having an important impact on bird communities (Sergio et al. 2003). However, in Mediterranean regions where rabbits may be widespread and abundant, Eagle Owl can turn into a predator specializing on rabbits. This top predator, with a vulnerable conservation status (Sergio et al. 2004), is widely distributed throughout Palaearctic Regions (Mikkola 1994, Penteriani 1996). It occurs in a variety of habitats, ranging from boreal forests to Mediterranean scrubland and steppes, including rocky and sandy deserts (Mikkola 1994, Penteriani 1996).

Field methods

At the age of 30–35 days, nestlings were fitted with a teflon ribbon backpack harness that carried a 30 g radio-transmitter (Biotrack Ltd, Wareham BH20 5AJ, Dorset, UK). The weight of the tags corresponded to less than 3% of the weight of the smallest adult male (1550 g) and 3.5% of the smallest fledgling weight (850 g) of our Eagle Owl population. Because at this time the young are still growing (Penteriani et al. 2005c), backpacks were adjusted so that the teflon ribbon could expand with increasing body size. We manipulated and marked owls under the Junta de Andalucía — Consejería de Medio Ambiente permits No. SCFFS-AFR/GGG RS-260/02 and SCFFS-AFR/CMM RS-1904/02. After 4 years of continuous radiotracking of 23 breeders and 74 floaters, we never recorded a possible adverse effect that could be directly attributed to backpacks on birds (Delgado & Penteriani, unpubl. data; see also Petty et al. 2004, Sunde 2006).

Based on earlier visits to the nests we estimated the age of the chicks (Penteriani et al. 2005c) and sexed them by molecular procedures using DNA extracted from blood (Griffiths et al. 1998). To determine individual physical condition we measured morphological, biometrical and biochemical parameters. Morphological and biometrical measurements were summarized into a body condition index estimated by a reduced major axis (RMA) regression (Green 2001), using log of both body mass (to the nearest 10 g, with 1 kg Pesola scales) and wing length (using a digital calliper, ± 0.1 mm). Higher values of the BCI represent higher-quality individuals (Green 2001). To obtain biochemical measurements, blood samples were collected and stored in tubes with heparin at 4°C until arrival at the laboratory, where they were centrifuged for 10 min at 4000 rpm and the plasma was separated and stored at -78°C. From plasma samples, cholesterol urea and total protein concentrations were determined using a spectrophotometer (Screenpoint 2, COR SRL; Ginestra Florentina, Italy), using commercial kits (BIOLABO). These biochemical parameters have been shown to be good indices of body condition in birds (e.g. Alonso-Alvarez et al. 2002).

We defined the different life history phases as follows. The PFDP started when the first juveniles left the nest. Because the majority of nests in our study area were on the ground or on small cliffs, this happened approximately at an age of 40–45 days (late March in our study area; see Penteriani et al 2005c). This period lasted until the beginning of natal dispersal. Natal dispersal started when the distance between successive moves became larger than the average distance travelled by each animal (Palomares et al. 2000, Delgado & Penteriani 2008). This happened at the end of August in our study area, when birds were 170 ± 20.5 days old (range 131–232 days).

During the PFDP we performed periods of intensive radiotracking two times per week during the whole night, from one hour before sunset to one hour after sunrise. In these radiotracking sessions we visited all the nests and radiolocated owlets from all family units simultaneously, with 1-hour time interval between successive individual locations. As fledglings are not very mobile and alternate movements with long resting stages (Delgado & Penteriani, unpubl. data), we suspect to have obtained similar results when taking more fixes per time unit. Night locations of radio-marked animals were carried out via biangulation with a 3-element hand-held Yagi-antenna connected to ICOM (ICR20) portable receivers ( www.icom.co.jp). Biangulations were analysed by ArcView 3.2 geographic information system (GIS) software.

Post-fledging paths analysis

CHARACTERISTICS OF MOVEMENTS, TERRITORY USE AND FAMILY UNIT INTERACTIONS

We divided the PFDP into 6 periods of 20 days, following Penteriani et al. (2005c) and Delgado & Penteriani (2007). To study the essential characteristics of movements, for each of the 20-day period we calculated the mean distance between successive owlet locations (i.e. mean step length), and the mean distance between the nest and each location point collected with an hour time fixed interval during the nights of radiotracking. To evaluate the interactions between siblings (i.e. family unit interactions) we estimated the between-sibling distances, that is the mean distance between owlets from the same family unit (by using individual locations recorded simultaneously). Finally, to understand how the fledglings explored surroundings of the nest, we estimated the post-fledging areas (hereafter PFAs) using 95% minimum convex polygons (Hayne 1949). These areas were calculated for each individual for each night; then, we computed the mean of these areas in each 20-day period.

SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL MOVEMENT PATTERNS

We estimated movement path tortuousity using fractal dimension, D. Fractal D is the continuous analogue of geometric dimensions, and ranges from a minimum of 1 for a straight line to a maximum of 2 when the path completely covers the plane. Fractal D was computed using the Fractal Mean estimator with the program Fractal 3.16 (Nams 2006a). We calculated an overall estimate of fractal D for each path, using the same range of scales for all movement paths (from 10 to 100 m). The upper limit was less than half the lengths of the longest paths (Halley et al. 2004). Using the same range of scales allowed us to compare fractal D among paths even though D varied with scale, thus avoiding the problems pointed out by Turchin (1996). Like most movement parameters, there is some correlation among fractal D, step length and home range size. However using the same range of scales for all animals ensured that fractal D is not totally dependent on either step length or range size. We calculated the average fractal D in each 20-day period, estimating values of confidence intervals from the variation among the paths. Error values are given as 95% confidence intervals.

We also analysed the movement mechanisms during the PFDP. This was done using the CRWDiff statistic, which measures the deviation in squared net distance traversed between the observed and correlated random walk (CRW) expected paths (Scaling test, Nams 2006b). That is, the index measures how similar the path is to a CRW. The Scaling test is based on the biological distinction that the mechanisms for unoriented movements act locally (i.e. at small spatial scale), while all oriented movements imply long-distant behavioural mechanisms. Although CRWDiff was initially designed in order to test for oriented movement by dispersing animals, we are using it simply as a general index to compare different paths. We treated each path as an individual sample, estimating the average CRWDiff values for each 20-day period, and estimating values of confidence intervals from variation among paths.

Statistical analyses

Repeated measurement mixed models were applied (PROC MIXED in SAS software; SAS Institute 2001) to analyse the variability in movement characteristics, space use and family unit interactions as a function of the 20-day period, sex and year. Because repeated measures were made for each owlet we considered individuals as subjects (SUBJECT statement in PROC MIXED), with nests as an additional random effect because they represent only a subsample of all potential territories (Littell et al. 1996, Revilla et al. 2002). We used the restricted maximum likelihood method (REML) to estimate all the unknown variance-covariance parameters (Jennrich & Schluchter 1986) and selected compound symmetry as the covariance structure that was the best fit, using the Akaike Information Criterion to measure model fit. Finally, statistical significance was considered to be P < 0.05.

Table 1.

Number of owl fledglings, total number of owlet locations, the average number of locations per individual, with SD and range, in each 20-day period used to analyse owlet movement patterns during the post-fledging dependence period. The total number of owlets was 41, but not all were recorded in each 20-day period.

t01_07.gif

The original fractal D was non-normal, but the logarithm of D—1 fitted a Gaussian distribution (Katz & George 1985). Then, we used Pearson's bivariate correlation to analyse the degree of tortuousity of the search paths among 20 days-periods.

RELATING MOVEMENT PATTERNS TO INDIVIDUAL QUALITY

To explore if changes in movement patterns were related to fledgling physical condition (that is, if individuals in better physical condition changed movement patterns sooner), we estimated the slope of the curves of both fractal D and CRWDiff against time. This was done for the 20-day periods of the 10 individuals who were continuously followed during the whole PFDP. Individuals that changed their movement patterns sooner should have curves with steeper slopes. Then, we used Generalized Linear Models (GLM, McCullagh & Neider 1989) to explore if these slope values were relate to fledgling physical condition. Each explanatory variable (physical condition parameters) and their interactions were fitted to the observed data using the GENMOD procedure of the SAS package (SAS Institute 2001).

RESULTS

Movement characteristics, territory use and family unit interactions

We obtained 1962 locations of the 41 tagged owlets (Table 1).

Average step length, distance from the nest, between-sibling distances and PFAs size increased with time throughout the post-fledging dependence period (Fig. 1). Step lengths were short (mean ± 95% CI: 343.6 ± 32.0 m) when juveniles left the nest and until 20 days after fledging. In the next 60 days, the distance travelled between successive moves significantly increased (GLIMMIX, F5 = 6.66, P < 0.0001; Fig. 1A), reaching the highest values close to dispersal (mean 734.2 ± 160.8 m). Also, juveniles were closer to the nest during the first 20-day period (334.8 ± 25.5 m), travelling further from the nest during the rest of the PFDP (GLIMMIX, F5 = 61.66, P < 0.0001), reaching a maximum distance just before dispersal (Fig. 1B). There were no significant effects on any of the movement characteristics of owlet sex, year, or their interactions (all P > 0.40).

Figure 1.

Movement characteristics of fledged Eagle Owls (n = 41) by 20-day periods during the post-fledging dependence period. Given are means and 95% confidence intervals. (A) Mean distance between successive nightly locations, (B) distance from the nest, (C) distances among siblings, and (D) size of post-fledging areas.

f01_07.eps

Between-sibling distances showed a significant interaction between the effects of sex and time (GLIMMIX, F1,5 = 28.73, P < 0.0001). Until 20 days after owlets left the nest, family units were closer together, independently of the sex of juveniles (mean 197.3 ± 30.3 m). Between-sibling distances increased with time as individuals became increasingly mobile (Fig. 1C). The closest proximity was between siblings of different sex (mean 237.8 ± 30.6 m), the next closest was between males (mean 289.9 ± 18.9 m), and the furthest was between females (mean 309.5 ± 24.9 m). Just before dispersal, family units seemed to dissolve, with a mean spacing between individuals of 613.4 ± 148.9 m. PFAs showed two main significant effects. PFAs varied between years with owlets having larger PFAs in 2004 (mean 0.4 ± 0.10 km2) than 2005 (mean 0.1 ± 0.04 km2) and with time since fledging: owlets increased the areas prospected from 0.16 ± 0.05 km2 when they left the nest to a maximum of 0.94 ± 0.28 km2 just before dispersal (GLIMMIX, F1,5 = 308.74, P < 0.0001; Fig. 1D). We did not detect any effects of sex on PFAs.

Scaling test for directed movement and fractal analysis

Path tortuousity significantly decreased with time since fledgling (r = -0.89, P < 0.05; Fig. 2A). Paths were more tortuous when owlets left the nest (mean 1.19; 95% CI: 1.12–1.27) than at the end of the dependence period (mean 1.09; 95% CI: 1.06–1.11). At the time that owlets travelled significantly farther distances, their movements described straighter paths.

The statistic CRWDiff was negative during the entire PFDP (Fig. 2B), indicating that the movement paths covered a shorter distance than a CRW would. The confidence intervals do not include zero, so juvenile paths during the dependence period were significantly unoriented.

Movement patterns and individual condition

We did not detect any significant effects of the condition parameters (body condition index, blood cholesterol urea and total blood protein concentrations: all P > 0.5) on slope of the curves of either fractal D or CRWDiff. That is, fledglings seemed to change their movement behaviour during the PFDP independently of their physical condition: individuals in better condition did not change their movement behaviour sooner during the PDFP.

Figure 2.

Change in movement paths of fledgling Eagle Owls (n = 41) during the post-fledging dependence period. (A) Scaling test applied to movement paths of young Eagle Owls during the PFDP. CRWDiff measures the distance travelled by the movement path as compared to a correlated random walk (CRW). (B) Path tortuousity (Fractal D; means and 95% confidence intervals) decreased with time throughout the post-fledging period. Given are means and 95% confidence intervals.

f02_07.eps

DISCUSSION

The analysis of fledgling movements allowed us to detect several notable behaviours during the PFDP. After leaving the nest, owlets moved with short steps, focusing their activities at, or very close to, the nest. Consequently, at this time, the PFAs prospected by young were limited to areas around the nest. After a few weeks, the movements of fledglings showed a marked change, with their movement trajectories longer than initially observed. Moreover, they were frequently located farther from their nests. This change in post-fledging behaviour may be probably due to the increased flying ability of owlets, as reported for other species (Belthoff & Ritchison 1989, Ferrer 1992, King & Belthoff 2001, Mínguez et al. 2001). In fact, when the young leave the nest (approximately 40–45 days): (a) remiges and rectrices are only 80% and 40% of their final length, respectively; (b) secondaries are still encased in 7–8 cm sheathings; and (c) wing coverts are only starting to emerge (Penteriani et al. 2005c). Because at this stage owlets cannot fly but rather walk and jump among rocks and brushes, the movement patterns we observed mainly reflect this form of displacement, i.e. that of a ‘terrestrial bird’ that cannot use its wings.

Although fledglings continued to expand their PFA size indefinitely until departing from the parent territories, most of PFAs included the nest within their activity area throughout the entire PFDP. Similar patterns reported by McClaren et al. (2005: Goshawks Accipiter gentilis), Wood et al. (1998: Bald Eagles Haliaeetus leucocephalus), and Belthoff & Ritchison (1989: Eastern Screech-Owls Megascops asio) suggest that nests still represent a focal point throughout the post-fledging period.

In the early PFDP, siblings tended to move together, with a constant distance between them during the first weeks. But, as independence neared, the distance increased progressively and family units seemed to partially dissolve. This is in accordance with Newton's (1979) idea that fledglings of raptors tend to perch apart, as also observed by Bustamante & Hiraldo (1990), Ceballos & Donázar (1990) and Bustamante (1994; but see O'Toole et al. 1999), but at later stages of PFDP only. Increased sibling distance can be due, in a non-mutually exclusive way, to conflict behaviours between juveniles or increased flight abilities.

Although inter-sibling distances increased during the entire dependence period, juveniles of different sexes tended to stay close together during the whole PFDP. Such a link among siblings during the whole PFDP does not necessarily break up after the start of dispersal: in contrast to other raptor species (e.g. Black Kites Milvus migrans, Bustamante & Hiraldo 1990 and Lesser Kestrels Falco naumanni, Bustamante & Negro 1994), some Eagle Owls move together during the first steps of dispersal (Delgado & Penteriani, unpubl. data).

Spatial and temporal movement patterns

The spatial and temporal movement patterns were not related with individual physical condition, showing all fledglings have a similar pattern in movement behaviour during the PDFP. This suggests that post-fledging movement behaviour may be a general ability acquired during this phase as the result of the combination of several traits, as follows. Our fledgling Eagle Owls showed unoriented movement (CRWDiff ≤ 0; Nams 2006b). This is not surprising for the following three reasons. (A) They are fed and protected by their parents. Consequently, they do not need to direct their movements towards specific shelters or foraging areas. (B) After leaving the nest, young are embedded in new surroundings that they have to learn, i.e. they move randomly during territory explorations. (C) Their flight abilities and perceptual range are not yet completely developed, so unoriented walks represent the best strategy to cover larger areas at small scales (Garcia et al. 2005). Moreover, during early PFDP, movement paths were more tortuous and became progressively straighter as independence approached. Since the paths were unoriented, we suggest that this increase in straightness may indicate an increase in their perceptual range (i.e. the maximum distances from which an animal can perceive the presence of a particular landscape element as such; Zollner 2000). That is, when animals are moving with an unoriented search strategy, the behavioural mechanisms governing movement are working at small spatial scales (Nams 2006b), and those small scales are determined by the perceptual range. Increasing the perceptual range would increase the natural step length of their movement path, resulting in straighter paths.

From early PFDP individuals are developing, day to day, their perceptual range (i.e. the ability to perceive habitat at a distance). Perceptual range has important ecological implications since it may determine the appropriate search strategy (Zollner & Lima 1999) and, consequently, influences both fledgling survival (i.e. their availability as future floaters of a population) and distribution patterns and dynamics of (meta) populations (Pulliam et al. 1992, Lima & Zollner 1996, Zollner 2000).

Finally, tortuous paths may have resulted from both incomplete growth and cognitive abilities (as perception and imperfect knowledge of the parental territory). With age, individuals increase in both flight and cognitive abilities, and perceptual range increases as fledglings become more familiar with their surroundings. This combination of traits enables them to search more rapidly and over larger areas, resulting in straighter movement paths. In fact, when their wings are fully developed, their displacements are over brushes, rocks and trees and not by walking and jumping among them. The existence of a trade-off between the relative path sinuosity and the size of the area searched has been also demonstrated by Doerr & Doerr (2004), which found that individuals who explored larger areas did so with less tortuousity.

Because the PFDP represents an intensive phase of experience and learning, and its influence may well shape fledglings' behavioural strategies during their successive, crucial phase, the natal dispersal, more information on the PFDP is needed to better understand avian breeding cycles and its consequences on individual survival.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

For their help with logistics we are grateful to P. Bartolommei, L. Campioni, R. Lourenço and G. Penteriani. The first draft was improved by the criticisms of A. Banks. Funding for this study was provided by a research project No. CGL2004-02780/BOS of Spanish Ministry of Education and Science and LICOR43 (Diego Zamora S.A.;  www.buho-ebd.com). During this work M.M. Delgado was supported by a doctoral grant of the Junta de Andalucía (Consejería de Educación y Ciencia). We manipulated and marked owls under the Junta de Andalucía — Consejería de Medio Ambiente permits No. SCFFS-AFR/GGG RS-260/02 and SCFFS-AFR/CMM RS-1904/02.

REFERENCES

1.

A.A.Y. Adams , S.K. Skagen & J.A. Savidge 2006. Modelling postfledging survival of lark buntings in response to ecological and biological factors. Ecology 87: 178–188. Google Scholar

2.

A. Aebischer , P. Nyffeler , S. Koch & R. Arlettaz 2005. Jugend-dispersion und Mortalität Schweizer Uhus (Bubo bubo) — ein aktueller Zwischenbericht. Ornithol. Anz. 44: 197–200. Google Scholar

3.

C. Alonso-Álvarez , M. Ferrer & A. Velando 2002. The plasmatic index of body condition in yellow-legged gulls Larus cachinnans: a food-controlled experiment. Ibis 144: 147–149. Google Scholar

4.

A. Amar , B.E. Arroyo & V. Bretagnolle 2000. Post-fledging dependence and dispersal in hacked and wild Montagu's harriers Cyrcus pygargus. Ibis 142: 21–28. Google Scholar

5.

H. Bailey & P. Thompson 2006. Quantitative analysis of bottlenose dolphin movement patterns and their relationship with foraging. J. Anim. Ecol. 75: 456–465. Google Scholar

6.

J.R. Belthoff & G. Ritchison 1989. Natal dispersal of Eastern Screech-Owls. Condor 91: 254–265. Google Scholar

7.

P.G. Blackwell 1997. Random diffusion models for animal movement. Ecol. Model. 100: 87–102. Google Scholar

8.

H.E. Bogner & G.A. Baldassarre 2002. Home range, movement, and nesting of Least Bitterns in Western New York. Wilson Bull. 114: 297–308. Google Scholar

9.

D.E. Bowler & T.G. Benton 2005. Causes and consequences of animal dispersal strategies: relating individual behaviour to spatial dynamics. Biol. Rev. 80: 205–225. Google Scholar

10.

J. Bustamante 1994. Behaviour of colonial Common Kestrels (Falco tinnunculus) during the postfledging dependence period in southwestern Spain. J. Raptor Res. 28: 79–83. Google Scholar

11.

J. Bustamante & F. Hiraldo 1990. Factors influencing family rupture and parent-offspring conflict in the Black Kite (Milvus migrans). Ibis 132: 58–67. Google Scholar

12.

J. Bustamante & J.J. Negro 1994. The post-fledging dependence period of the lesser kestrel (Falco naumanni) in southwestern spain. J. Raptor Res. 28: 158–163. Google Scholar

13.

O. Ceballos & J.A. Donázar 1990. Parent-offspring conflict during the postfledging period in the Egyptian Vulture Neophron percnopterus (Aves, Accipitridae). Ethology 85: 225–235. Google Scholar

14.

J.J. Chris , K.L. Parker , D.C. Heard & M.P. Gillingham 2002. Movement parameters of ungulates and scale-specific responses to the environment. J. Anim. Ecol. 71: 225–235. Google Scholar

15.

T.O. Crist , S. Guerting , J.A. Wiens & B.T. Milne 1992. Animal movements in heterogeneous landscapes: an experiment with Eleodes beetles in shortgrass prairie. Funct. Ecol. 6: 536–544. Google Scholar

16.

M.M. Delgado & V. Penteriani 2007. Vocal behaviour and neighbour spatial arrangement during vocal displays in Eagle owl (Bubo bubo). J. Zool. 271:3–10. Google Scholar

17.

M.M. Delgado & V. Penteriani 2008. Behavioural states help translate dispersal movements into spatial distribution patterns of floaters. Am. Nat. 172: 475–485. Google Scholar

18.

M. Delibes , P. Gaona & P. Ferreras 2001. Effects of an attractive sink leading into maladaptative habitat selection. Am. Nat. 158: 277–285. Google Scholar

19.

V.A.J. Doerr & E.D. Doerr 2004. Fractal analysis can explain individual variation in dispersal search paths. Ecology 85: 1428–1438. Google Scholar

20.

M. Ferrer 1992. Regulation of the period of postfledging dependence period in the Spanish Imperial Eagle Aquila adalberti. Ibis 134: 128–133. Google Scholar

21.

F. Garcia , P. Carrère , J.F. Soussana & R. Baumont 2005. Characterisation by fractal analysis of foraging paths of ewes grazing heterogeneous swards. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 93: 19–37. Google Scholar

22.

B.J. Goodwing & L. Farhig 2002. Effect of landscape structure on the movement behaviour of a specialized goldenrod bettle, Thrirhabda borealis. Can. J. Zoolog. 80: 24–35. Google Scholar

23.

A. Green 2001. Mass/length residuals: measures of body condition or generators of spurious results? Ecology 82: 1473–1483. Google Scholar

24.

R. Griffiths , M.C. Double , K. Orr & R.J.G. Dawson 1998. A DNA test to sex most birds. Mol. Ecol. 7: 1071–1075. Google Scholar

25.

J.M. Halley , S. Hartley , A.S. Kallimanis , W.E. Kunin , J.J. Lennon & S.P. Sgardelis 2004. Uses and abuses of fractal methodology in ecology. Ecol. Lett. 7: 254–271. Google Scholar

26.

I. Hanski 1999. Metapopulation Ecology. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Google Scholar

27.

D.W. Hayne 1949. Calculation of home range size. J. Mammal. 30: 1–18. Google Scholar

28.

M. Heino , V. Kaitala , E. Ranta & J. Lindström 1997. Synchronous dynamics and rates of extincion in spatially structured populations. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264: 481–486. Google Scholar

29.

R.A. Hylton , P.C. Frederick , T.E. De La Fuente & M.G. Spalding 2006. Effects of nestling health on postfledging survival of woods storks. Condor 108: 97–106. Google Scholar

30.

R.I. Jennrich & M.D. Schluchter 1986. Unbalanced repeated-measures models with structured covariance matrices. Biometrics 42: 805–820. Google Scholar

31.

P.M. Kareiva & N. Shigesada 1983. Analyzing insect movement as a correlated random walk. Oecologia 56: 234–238. Google Scholar

32.

M.J. Katz & E. George 1985. Fractals and the analysis of growth paths. B. Math. Biol. 47: 273–286. Google Scholar

33.

E.L. Kershner , J.W. Walk & E.R. Warner 2004. Postfledging movements and survival of juvenile eastern meadowlarks (Sturnella magna) in Illinois. Auk 121: 1146–1154. Google Scholar

34.

R.A. King & J.R. Belthoff 2001. Post-fledging dispersal of Burrowing Owls in southwestern Idaho: characterization of movements and use of satellite burrows. Condor 103: 118–123. Google Scholar

35.

S.L. Lima & P.A. Zollner 1996. Towards a behavioral ecology of ecological landscapes. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11: 131–135. Google Scholar

36.

R.C. Littell , G.A. Milliken , W.W. Stroup & R.D. Wolfinger 1996. SAS System for mixed models. SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA. Google Scholar

37.

R.F. Lourenço 2006. The food habits of Eurasian Eagle-Owls in Southern Portugal. J. Raptor Res. 40: 297–300. Google Scholar

38.

L. Marchesi , F. Sergio & P. Pedrini 2002. Costs and benefits of breeding in human-altered landscapes for the Eagle Owl Bubo bubo. Ibis 144: 164–177. Google Scholar

39.

J.A. Martinez , D. Serrano & I. Zuberogoitia 2003. Predictive model of habitat preferences for the Eurasian Eagle-Owl Bubo bubo: a multiscale approach. Ecography 26: 21–28. Google Scholar

40.

E.L. McClaren , P.L. Kennedy & D.D. Doyle 2005. Northern goshawk (Acciptier gentilis laingi) postfledging areas on Vancouver Island, British Columbia. J. Raptor Res. 39: 253–263. Google Scholar

41.

P. McCullagh & J.A. Neider 1989. Generalised Linear Modelling. Chapman & Hall, London. Google Scholar

42.

H. Mikkola 1994. Eagle Owl. Birds of Europe: their conservation status. In: G.M. Tucker & M.F. Heath (eds) BirdLife Conservation Series, 3. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 326–327. Google Scholar

43.

E. Mínguez , E. Angulo & V. Siebering 2001. Factors influencing length of the post-fledging period and timing of dispersal in Bonelli's eagle (Hieraatus fasciatus) in southwestern Spain. J. Raptor Res. 35: 228–234. Google Scholar

44.

J.S. Monrós , E.J. Belda & E. Barba 2002. Post-fledging survival of individual great tits: the effect of hatching date and fledging mass. Oikos 99: 481–488. Google Scholar

45.

M.C. Myers & C. Vaughan 2004. Movement and behavior of scarlet macaws (Ara macao) during the post-fledging dependence period: implications for in situ versus ex situ management. Biol. Conserv. 118: 411–420. Google Scholar

46.

V. Nams 2006a. Improving accuracy and precision in estimating fractal dimension of animal movement paths. Acta Biotheor. 54: 1–11. Google Scholar

47.

V. Nams 2006b. Detecting oriented movement of animals. Anim. Behav. 72: 1197–1203. Google Scholar

48.

I. Newton 1979. Population ecology of raptors. Buteo Books, Vermillion, SD, USA. Google Scholar

49.

L.T. O'Toole , P.L. Kennedy , R.L. Knight & L.C. McEwen 1999. Postfledging behavior of Golden Eagles. Wilson Bull. 111: 472–477. Google Scholar

50.

P. Palomares , M. Delibes , J.M. Fedriani , J. Calzada & E. Revilla 2000. Iberian lynx in a fragmented landscape: predispersal, dispersal, and postdispersal habitats. Conserv. Biol. 14: 809–818. Google Scholar

51.

V. Penteriani 1996. The Eagle Owl. Calderini Edagricole. Google Scholar

52.

V. Penteriani , M. Gallardo , P. Roche & H. Cazassus 2001. Effects of landscape spatial structure and composition on the settlement of the Eagle Owl Bubo bubo in a Mediterranean habitat. Ardea 89: 331–340. Google Scholar

53.

V. Penteriani , M. Gallardo & P. Roche 2002. Landscape structure and food supply affect Eagle Owl Bubo bubo density and breeding performance: a case of intra-population heterogeneity. J. Zool. 257: 365–372. Google Scholar

54.

V. Penteriani , F. Otalora & M. Ferrer 2005a. Floater survival affects population persistence. The role of prey availability and environmental stochasticity. Oikos 108: 523–534. Google Scholar

55.

V. Penteriani , F. Otalora , F. Sergio & M. Ferrer 2005b. Environmental stochasticity in dispersal areas can explain the “mysterious” disappearance of breeding populations. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 272: 1265–1269. Google Scholar

56.

V. Penteriani , M.M. Delgado , C. Maggio , A. Aradis & F. Sergio 2005c. Development of chicks and pre-dispersal behaviour of young in the Eagle Owl Bubo bubo. Ibis 147: 155–168. Google Scholar

57.

V. Penteriani , F. Otalora & M. Ferrer 2006. Floater dynamics can explain positive patterns of density-dependent fecundity in animal populations. Am. Nat. 168: 697–703. Google Scholar

58.

S.J. Petty , B.M. Appleby , C.F. Cles & R. Julliard 2004. The long-term effect of fitting back-mounted radio tags to juvenile tawny owls Strix aluco. Wildlife Biol. 10: 161–170. Google Scholar

59.

H.R. Pulliam , J.B. Dunning Jr. & J. Liu 1992. Population dynamics in complex landscapes: a case study. Ecol. Appl. 2: 165–177. Google Scholar

60.

J.H. Rappole 1995. The ecology of migrant birds. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington. Google Scholar

61.

E. Revilla & F. Palomares 2002. Spatial organization, group living and ecological correlates in low-density populations of Eurasian badgers, Meles meles. J. Anim. Ecol. 71: 497–512. Google Scholar

62.

SAS Institute. 2001. SAS/STAT software: changes and enhancements, version 8.2. SAS Publishing. Google Scholar

63.

F. Sergio , L. Marchesi & P. Pedrini 2003. Spatial refugia and the coexistence of a diurnal raptor with its intraguild owl predator. J. Anim. Ecol. 72: 232–245. Google Scholar

64.

F. Sergio , L. Marchesi , P. Pedrini , M. Ferrer & V. Penteriani 2004. Electrocution alters the distribution and density of a top predator, the eagle owl Bubo bubo. J. Appl. Ecol. 41: 836–845. Google Scholar

65.

K.A. Sullivan 1989. Predation and starvation: Age-specific mortality in juvenile juncos (Junco phaenotus). J. Anim. Ecol. 58: 275–286. Google Scholar

66.

P. Sunde 2005. Predators control post-fledging mortality in tawny owls, Strix aluco. Oikos 110: 461–472. Google Scholar

67.

P. Sunde 2006. Effects of Backpack Radio Tags on Tawny Owls. J. Wildlife Manage. 70: 594–599. Google Scholar

68.

P. Sunde & S.M. Redpath 2006. Combining information from range use and habitat selection: sex-specific spatial responses to habitat fragmentation in tawny owls Strix aluco. Ecography 29: 152–158. Google Scholar

69.

P. Turchin 1996. Fractal analyses of animal movement — a critique. Ecology 77: 2086–2090. Google Scholar

70.

P. Turchin 1998. Quantitative analysis of movement: measuring and modeling population redistribution in animals and plants. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, USA. Google Scholar

71.

R.A. Vásquez , L.A. Ebensperger & F. Bozinovic 2002. The influence of habitat on travel speed, intermittent locomotion, and vigilance in a diurnal rodent. Behav. Ecol. 13: 182–187. Google Scholar

72.

P.J. Weatherhead & S.B. McRae 1990. Brood care in American Robins: Implications for mixed reproductive strategies by females. Anim. Behav. 39: 1179–1188. Google Scholar

73.

J.A. Wiens , T.O. Crist , K.A. With & B.T. Milne 1995. Fractal patterns of insect movement in microlandscape mosaics. Ecology 76: 663–666. Google Scholar

74.

J.A. Wiens , R.L. Schooley & R.D.J. Weeks 1997. Patchy landscapes and animal movements: Do beetles percolate? Oikos 78: 257–264. Google Scholar

75.

J.D. Wiens , R.T. Reynolds & B.R. Noon 2006. Juvenile movement and natal dispersal of Northern Goshawks in Arizona. Condor 108: 253–269. Google Scholar

76.

K.A. With & T.O. Crist 1995. Critical thresholds in species' responses to landscape structure. Ecology 76: 2446–2459. Google Scholar

77.

P.B. Wood , M.W. Collopy & C.M. Sekerak 1998. Postfledging nest dependence period for Bald Eagles in Florida. J. Wildl. Manage. 62: 333–339. Google Scholar

78.

B.J. Worton 1987. A review of models of home range for animal movement. Ecol. Model. 38: 227–298. Google Scholar

79.

P.A. Zollner 2000. Comparing the landscape level perceptual abilities of forest sciurids in fragmented agricultural land-scapes. Landscape Ecol. 15: 523–533. Google Scholar

80.

P.A. Zollner & S.L. Lima 1999. Search strategies for landscape-level interpatch movements. Ecology 80: 1019–1030. Google Scholar

Appendices

SAMENVATTING

Er is veel onderzoek verricht naar de wijze waarop dieren zieh in de ruimte verplaatsen. Er is echter weinig bekend over de bewegingen direct na het uitvliegen van jonge vogels, een periode waarin ze nog afhankelijk van de ouders zijn. Deze periode is belangrijk als voorbereiding op de zelfstandigheid van de jongen. Naarmate de vogels handiger worden en hun omgeving beter leren kennen, gaan zij zich anders gedragen. Het onderhavige onderzoek in de Sierra Norte bij Sevilla, Spanje, gaat in op de verplaatsingen van 41 pas uitgevlogen Oehoes Bubo bubo die met een radiozendertje waren uitgerust. Direct na het uitvliegen bleven de vogels in de buurt van het nest, waarbij ze zieh vooral huppend en klauterend verplaatsten. Binnen een paar weken leerden ze echter vliegen en begonnen ze steeds sneller en verder weg te bewegen. Na ruim drie maanden werden de jongen zelfstandig en verlieten het ouderlijk territorium definitief. Naarmate de vogels zich sneller bewogen, verplaatsten ze zieh meer in een rechte lijn. De manier waarop de jonge uilen zieh verplaatsten, was niet afhankelijk van hun lichaamsgrootte of van conditie. De eerste 20 dagen na het uitvliegen bleven de jonge uilen dicht bij elkaar, maar snel daarna namen de onderlinge afstanden toe tot enkele honderden meters. (DH)

María del Mar Delgado, Vincenzo Penteriani, and Vilis O. Nams "How Fledglings Explore Surroundings from Fledging to Dispersal. A Case Study with Eagle Owls Bubo bubo," Ardea 97(1), 7-15, (1 April 2009). https://doi.org/10.5253/078.097.0102
Received: 14 June 2008; Accepted: 1 December 2008; Published: 1 April 2009
KEYWORDS
Bubo bubo
fractal analysis
path search strategies
perceptual range
post-fledging dependence period
Back to Top